Logtime: The Subjective Scale of Life

The Logarithmic Time Perception Hypothesis



By James Main Kenney







Time scarcer? Years getting shorter? Want an explanation? Logtime is the cognitive hypothesis that our age is our basis for estimating time intervals, resulting in a perceived shrinking of our years as we grow older. A simple mathematical analysis shows that our time perception should be logarithmic, giving us a subjective scale of life very different from that of the calendar. Our perception of aging seems to follow the same (Weber-Fechner) law as our perception of physical stimuli.

DN      CONTENTS     UP
Ticking away the moments that make up a dull day
You fritter and waste the hours in an offhand way.
...You are young and life is long and there is time to kill today
And then one day you find ten years have got behind you.
...Every year is getting shorter; never seem to find the time...

--
"Time" from The Dark Side of the Moon: Pink Floyd

tempus fugit When you've grown up, my dears,
and are as old as I,
you'll often ponder on the years
that roll so swiftly by,
my dears,
that roll so swiftly by...

--
"Toyland" from Babes in Toyland:
Glen MacDonough & Victor Herbert
time flies

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Problems of Time Perception



It's common knowledge that our perception of the passage of time can be influenced by psychological factors: time flies when we're busy, but really drags when we're waiting. (Stare at a clock and wait for a minute to pass. Or wait for the commercials to end, or for Windows to load!) These are generally short term experiences, but what of long periods of time such as years? Is there something other than transient psychological factors affecting our time perception?

We usually think about the years of our lives in terms of decades: our teens, twenties, thirties, etc. This is an implicitly linear view: that all our years are equal; that clock time is our time, through which we move at a uniform pace.

This simple picture, however, doesn't square with our perceptions as we age. By our middle years, at least, most of us have become aware that something is amiss, that a very slow but profound change has been sneaking up on us: the years that formerly crawled are now racing by. Where are the long, leisurely summers we knew as children? If it seemed forever to get through the fifth grade, what happened to last year? Why do we now seem so rushed by life? Where are all the things we wanted to accomplish, but never seemed to find the time for?

There is another clue that our lives are not running in a linear, clocklike fashion: when we try to remember back to the earliest years of our childhood, they seem incredibly distant, like a far horizon that always recedes as we attempt to approach it. Why should we find it so much harder to remember the first few years of life than to remember later years, even after a longer time? And why do parents see their children growing up so much faster than they did?

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Logtime: The Logarithmic Explanation



Since the linear view of time perception seems inadequate, it is reasonable to look for a non-linear alternative. The observations we make about the apparent shrinkage of our years as we age strongly suggest a logarithmic scale: stretched out at the low end and compressed at the high end.

The fundamental importance of the logarithm has suggested the term "Logtime" to succinctly refer to the cognitive hypothesis discussed here. (The reader who cringes at the very mention of the word "logarithm" need have no fear: no tables of logarithms will be used here and no math knowledge, except for the optional Appendix , will be required!)

Another term may usefully be coined here to denote this general field of study: "psychochronometry", the psychology of time estimation. Logtime is a psychochronometric model, i.e., an attempted mathematical simulation of subjective human temporal experiences. The logarithmic scale of time perception presented by this model may be only a rough approximation of actual human perception, but is probably a much closer one than the linear scale usually assumed.

That our time perception should be logarithmic can be easily rationalized (although proving it is a different matter!). The simple premise of Logtime, from which the logarithmic relationship can be derived (see Appendix ), is that the human mind judges the length of a long period of time, such as a year, by comparing it with current age. For example, a year adds 10% to the life of a ten-year-old, but only 5% to that of a twenty-year-old. For the twenty-year-old, two years are required to add 10%.

The Logtime hypothesis is that it is this percentage that we perceive, not the years themselves: to the twenty-year-old, two years will seem to pass as quickly as one year will seem to the ten-year-old. Similarly, three years to a thirty-year-old and four years to a forty-year-old, etc., will seem to pass equally fast. (This argument was recently found to have been used, comparing a "child of 10" with a "man of 50", by Sorbonne professor of philosophy Paul Janet, date unknown but quoted in an 1890 book by the eminent Harvard philosopher and psychology pioneer
William James , who seemed to accept the description but added his own explanation of an underlying psychological cause which would be difficult to analyze quantitatively.)

The Logtime hypothesis is consistent with the widely accepted description of the perception of physical stimuli commonly referred to as the "Weber-Fechner law" . For time perception, clock time (calendar age) is the "stimulus". Weber-Fechner has been found to be only an approximation over a limited stimulus range, and this would probably be the case for aging perception if objective measurements were possible.

The older we become, the faster we seem to age or, conversely, the shorter the years seem to be. Mathematically, this relationship is said to be either logarithmic or exponential, depending on which variable is used as the reference: the length of the years seems to shrink logarithmically if we regard our subjective aging as uniform, while the speed of passage of these years seems to increase exponentially if we regard the years as being of equal length. Using other terminology, our sense of aging follows an arithmetic progression while the corresponding calendar years follow a geometric progression.

The Logtime hypothesis probably applies to all time intervals, not just years and seasons. Our days and hours should be similarly shrinking as we age, but short-term psychological factors tend to dominate, making the shrinkage less obvious.

When we are young, the changing nature of our lives tends to obscure the shrinking years: the twenty-year-old rarely thinks about how life was at age 10; life at 20 is filled with different activities and concerns, and it is the future that dominates reverie. (The twenty-year-old may be loath to admit to even having been 10, let alone suffer the remembrance of it!)

It is only after life becomes more settled and routine that we become more retrospective, and only then do we have an easier basis for comparing the years. However, we tend to adjust to our changing time scale, and our declining physical abilities tends to conceal the nature of the underlying change: since we see ourselves as "slowing down", we accept that life around us seems to go faster. (Of course, if you accelerate down the road, the passing scenery also accelerates!*)

Some
authors have identified the change in time perception with changing metabolic rate, but is there enough change in the latter to account for the former? If not, it may be irrelevant to the "clock" the human mind uses.

*Analogies between time perception changes and spatial motion can be confusing and ambiguous since we don't actually move through time (nor does time move through us): we merely exist as four-dimensional bodies in a four-dimensional space-time continuum (ignoring the many new dimensions being added by modern physics). In a sense, only our consciousness can be said to "move" as it perceives three-dimensional spatial cross-sections in sequence along the time axis. Actually, nothing can really move in space-time: things just exist, including the four-dimensional physical structure underlying our consciousness. (But what is existence? What is consciousness?... What is this headache?!)

DN      CONTENTS     UP

The Scale of Life



It will be convenient, though perhaps depressing, to define equally perceived units of time to replace the decades we conventionally use to pace our lives. A consequence of the logarithmic function is that it is the ratio of the years defining an interval of time that we use to judge the duration of that interval, not the absolute magnitudes of those years. (See Appendix .)

For example, using the simplest ratio, 2: the years from ages 10 to 20 seem to pass at the same rate as the years from 20 to 40, or 40 to 80. The starting age is arbitrary: 8 to 16, 16 to 32, and 32 to 64 are also of equal subjective duration, and are all perceived as being the same as 10 to 20!

The term commonly applied to ratios of 2 is the musical one of "octave" (ignoring its Latin root meaning "8"). (Our perceptions of the pitch and amplitude of musical tones are logarithmic; so, apparently, is our perception of time.) The octave is the most obvious Logtime replacement for the decade, although it has no fixed connection to our base-10 number system. All octaves are equally spaced on a log scale, just as decades are equally spaced on a linear scale.

Going back in time, the octaves from ages 4 to 8, 2 to 4, and 1 to 2, etc., should have seemed equally long, although the logarithmic description may no longer be a good approximation near birth, and can certainly not be quantified by observation!

There is a question of origin: when is "zero"? Is it birth, conception, sometime in between, or even a time after birth when long-term memories may actually start to form? (There are cultures that consider the newborn to be a year old.) Since a true log scale has no origin, no zero, going back toward age "zero" is a limitless process: it would require an infinite number of octaves. This offers a plausible explanation for our difficulties in remembering our earliest years if our memories decay in proportion to octave separation. (If forgetfulness was linear, i.e., uniform through the years, why should a twenty-year old not be able to recall his first year of life as easily as a forty-year old recalls his twenty-first year?) (Of course, for those of us with really poor memories, the difference may not be all that great!)

On a log scale, the reference replacing the zero of a linear scale is unity. Age "1" is five octaves removed from age "32", six octaves from "64", etc., however "1" (or "zero") is defined. A mathematically correct choice for "1" would be possible only if these temporal effects were measurable and predictable.

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Logarithmic Lifetimes



Since the starting age of an octave is arbitrary, we can scale our lives logarithmically in various ways. The series of numbers on each line below all define the end points of (equally-perceived) octave age intervals. Note that the even spacing of subjective ages on a logarithmic scale results in an exponential growth of the objective (clock) age, an octave representing a growth of 100% per step:

   1      2      4      8     16     32     64    128
   1.0    2.1    4.1    8.2   16.5   33     66   (132)
   1.1    2.1    4.2    8.5   17     34     68   (136)
   1.1    2.2    4.4    8.8   17.5   35     70   (140)
   1.1    2.2    4.5    9     18     36     72   (144)
   1.2    2.3    4.6    9.2   18.5   37     74   (148)
   1.2    2.4    4.8    9.5   19     38     76   (152)
   1.2    2.4    4.9    9.8   19.5   39     78   (156)
   1.2    2.5    5     10     20     40     80   (160)
   1.3    2.6    5.1   10.2   20.5   41     82   (164)
   1.3    2.6    5.2   10.5   21     42     84   (168)
   1.3    2.7    5.4   10.8   21.5   43     86   (172)
   1.4    2.8    5.5   11     22     44     88   (176)
   1.4    2.8    5.6   11.2   22.5   45     90   (180)
   1.4    2.9    5.8   11.5   23     46     92   (184)
   1.5-   2.9    5.9   11.8   23.5   47     94   (188)
   1.5    3      6     12     24     48     96   (192)
   1.5+   3.1    6.1   12.2   24.5   49     98   (196)
   1.6    3.1    6.2   12.5   25     50    100   (200)
   1.6    3.2    6.4   12.8   25.5   51    102   (204)
   1.6    3.2    6.5   13     26     52    104   (208)
   1.7    3.3    6.6   13.2   26.5   53    106   (212)
   1.7    3.4    6.8   13.5   27     54    108   (216)
   1.7    3.4    6.9   13.8   27.5   55    110   (220)
   1.8    3.5    7     14     28     56    112   (224)
   1.8    3.6    7.1   14.2   28.5   57    114   (228)
   1.8    3.6    7.2   14.5   29     58    116   (232)
   1.8    3.7    7.4   14.8   29.5   59    118   (236)
   1.9    3.8    7.5   15     30     60    120   (240)
   1.9    3.8    7.6   15.2   30.5   61    122   (244)
   1.9    3.9    7.8   15.5   31     62    124   (248)
   2.0    3.9    7.9   15.8   31.5   63    126   (252)
   2      4      8     16     32     64    128   (256)
Any (horizontally) adjacent pair of ages defines an octave of life, and each octave should seem equally long. The numbers in parentheses represent ages greater than 128, which are not physically attainable at present; they are the theoretical bounds on the "broken octaves" that conclude our lives.

On any of the above lines of numbers, find a number closest to your age. On the same line will be the numbers bounding the octaves of your life. Think back to the ages at the left: do they seem equally spaced, i.e., does each interval seem to have been equally "long"? Do they represent equally important stages in your life? (You may not remember how important the earliest octaves really were!) How many numbers are at the right? If your next octave is a "broken" one, contemplate your fortune in having survived all the octaves at the left; many have not.

The course octave spacing of the above series can be relieved to any extent desired by geometric interpolation, which produces numbers that are still evenly spaced on a logarithmic scale. For example, the following have adjacent numbers that (before rounding) differ by a factor of the square root of 2 (1.414...), the "half octave", i.e., the exponential growth in clock age is a little over 41% per step:

   1      1.4
   2      2.8
   4      5.6
   8     11
  16     23
  32     45
  64     91
 128

   0.6    0.9
   1.2    1.8
   2.5    3.5+
   5      7.1
  10     14
  20     28
  40     57
  80    113
(160)
Interpolating again (before rounding) produces the following "quarter octave" series (as with the above, one series is based on an exact 1, and the other on an exact 10):

   1      1.2    1.4    1.7
   2      2.4    2.8    3.4
   4      4.8    5.7    6.7
   8      9.5+  11     13
  16     19     23     27
  32     38     45     54
  64     76     91    108
 128

   0.6    0.7    0.9    1.1
   1.2    1.5-   1.8    2.1
   2.5    3.0    3.5+   4.2
   5      5.9    7.1    8.4
  10     12     14     17
  20     24     28     34
  40     48     57     67
  80     95    113   (135)
Another interpolation and rounding produces the "eighth octave" series, with horizontally adjacent numbers differing by a factor of the eighth root of 2. (On all of these interpolated-octave tables, vertically adjacent numbers differ by a factor of 2.) Note the quasi-linear stretches, and the one-year spacing of the teen years that may serve as a convenient basis for comparison with other stages of life:

   1      1.1    1.2    1.3    1.4    1.5+   1.7    1.8
   2      2.2    2.4    2.6    2.8    3.1    3.4    3.7
   4      4.4    4.8    5.2    5.7    6.2    6.7    7.3
   8      8.7    9.5+  10     11     12     13     15
  16     17     19     21     23     25     27     29
  32     35     38     41     45     49     54     59
  64     70     76     83     91     99    108    117
 128

   0.6    0.7    0.7    0.8    0.9    1.0    1.1    1.1
   1.2    1.4    1.5-   1.6    1.8    1.9    2.1    2.3
   2.5    2.7    3.0    3.2    3.5+   3.9    4.2    4.6
   5      5.5-   5.9    6.5-   7.1    7.7    8.4    9.2
  10     11     12     13     14     15     17     18
  20     22     24     26     28     31     34     37
  40     44     48     52     57     62     67     73
  80     87     95    104    113    123   (135)
Musicians and music lovers may like to "scale" their lives using twelve steps per octave to match the chromatic (equal tempered) half-tone musical scale. Horizontally adjacent numbers differ by a factor of the twelfth root of 2 (1.059463...), i.e., there is an exponential growth of just under 6% per step. (Those who are into numerology as well as music may like to try finding a key with the accidentals best matching the years when they were particularly "sharp" or "flat"! Could this become a new occult fad?)

   1     1.1   1.1   1.2   1.3   1.3   1.4   1.5-  1.6   1.7   1.8   1.9
   2     2.1   2.2   2.4   2.5+  2.7   2.8   3.0   3.2   3.4   3.6   3.8
   4     4.2   4.5-  4.8   5.0   5.3   5.7   6.0   6.3   6.7   7.1   7.6
   8     8.5-  9.0   9.5+ 10.1  10.7  11.3  12.0  12.7  13.5- 14    15
  16    17    18    19    20    21    23    24    25    27    29    30
  32    34    36    38    40    43    45    48    51    54    57    60
  64    68    72    76    81    85    91    96   102   108   114   121
 128

   0.6   0.7   0.7   0.7   0.8   0.8   0.9   0.9   1.0   1.1   1.1   1.2
   1.2   1.3   1.4   1.5-  1.6   1.7   1.8   1.9   2.0   2.1   2.2   2.4
   2.5   2.6   2.8   3.0   3.1   3.3   3.5+  3.7   4.0   4.2   4.5-  4.7
   5     5.3   5.6   5.9   6.3   6.7   7.1   7.5-  7.9   8.4   8.9   9.4
  10    10.6  11.2  11.9  12.6  13.3  14    15    16    17    18    19
  20    21    22    24    25    27    28    30    32    34    36    38
  40    42    45    48    50    53    57    60    63    67    71    76
  80    85    90    95   101   107   113   120   127  (135)
It's not essential to base an age series directly on the octave, which can be identified on any log scale. Any number can be used for the ratio of adjacent entries. For example, we can make both 1 and 10 (and 100, also) exact by using a ratio of the n-th root of 10. Using the tenth root of 10 produces ten steps per factor of 10. This is commonly used in engineering (as the "decibel") to express power ratios. Every third number differs by almost a factor of 2 to a close approximation. (Power very nearly doubles for three-decibel steps.) (Ages below 10 can be obtained by moving the decimal point to the left.)

  10    13    16    20    25    32    40    50    63    79
 100   126  (158)
Interpolating to give finer resolution (twenty steps per factor of 10) produces ratios which, for voltage or current, etc., correspond to decibel steps. (Six-decibel steps very nearly double voltage or current, quadrupling power.)

  10    11    13    14    16    18    20    22    25    28
  32    35    40    45    50    56    63    71    79    89
 100   112   126  (141)
For an even finer resolution, we can use thirty steps per factor of 10:

  10    11    12    13    14    15    16    17    18    20
  22    23    25    27    29    32    34    37    40    43
  46    50    54    58    63    68    74    79    86    93
 100   108   117   126  (136)
Using forty steps per factor of 10:

  10    10.6  11.2  11.9  12.6  13.3  14    15    16    17
  18    19    20    21    22    24    25    27    28    30
  32    33    35    38    40    42    45    47    50    53
  56    60    63    67    71    75    79    84    89    94
 100   106   112   119   126  (133)
Finally, using fifty steps per factor of 10, for an exponential growth of about 4.7% per step:

  10    10.5- 11.0  11.5- 12.0  12.6  13.2  13.8  14.5- 15.1
  15.8  16.6  17.4  18    19    20    21    22    23    24
  25    26    28    29    30    32    33    35    36    38
  40    42    44    46    48    50    52    55    58    60
  63    66    69    72    76    79    83    87    91    95
 100   105   110   115   120   126  (132)
On any of the above tables, compare the spacing in the preteen years (moving the decimal point where required) and in the middle years with the spacing in the teens and twenties: how stretched out is childhood, and how compressed is middle age! The even-greater compression of old age makes that period seem very short, which brings us to a discussion of...
DN      CONTENTS     UP

The Last Judgement



Is that old saying "life is short" starting to make sense? Since we seem to be hurtling toward oblivion at an accelerating pace, an obvious question is: how much time do we have left on our subjective scale, i.e., how much longer will life seem to last? The above age tables are course for the later years, making estimates difficult.

At the risk of further depressing the reader (who must now be fully aware of the disturbing nature of this material!), a simple way to more precisely judge the time remaining is to perform the following calculations:
1. Assume an age to which you may reasonably expect to live (e.g., 80).

2. Divide this assumed age of death by your present age (if you are 40, then 80/40 = 2).

3. Divide your present age by this number (40/2 = 20).

4. The result is a "reference age" (20) as subjectively remote in your past as your assumed age of death (80) is in your future. Consider the years from that point in your life (age 20) to the present (age 40): the time you have left (40 to 80) should seem about as long.

5. Your present age is the geometric mean of the reference age and the assumed age of death, and becomes closer to the arithmetic mean as your present age approaches the latter. In old age, therefore, you can assume linearity: each future year will seem almost as long as each past year back to the reference age. (In other words, the worst is over!)
The basis for this calculation is, of course, the same fundamental property of a log scale that makes all octaves equal: all equal ratios of numbers are equally spaced. Since the ages involved will generally not have integral ratios, use of a calculator is recommended, especially for repeated calculations:

On a calculator, enter your age and square it. (Multiply it by itself if a "squaring" key is not provided.) Save the result in memory. Divide by the age you expect to reach in order to find the past "reference" age. Recall from memory the stored square of your present age and divide by a different age of death assumption. Doing this several times will give you a feel for the subjective time you have left. This could make a great party game! (Then again...?) (Well, maybe for an "Addams Family" party!)

You can also reverse the computation by dividing the square of your age by some past age that you may particularly recall and have a feel for the time from then till now. Will you live long enough to experience a similar future interval?

Those who place their greatest trust in computers may like to use the following BASIC program (using only integers); it will run under either GW-BASIC or QBASIC in MS-DOS computers (as LOGTIME.BAS), and should also run under most other versions of BASIC. (For the Tandy 100/102/200 and the NEC 8201A/8300 substitute MENU for SYSTEM and load as LOGTIM.DO.)
0 CLS:PRINT:PRINT" LOGTIME by James Main Kenney 1996
1 PRINT:INPUT"What is your present age";A
2 INPUT"To what age do you expect to live";L:IF L<=A THEN 2 ELSE PRINT"The time from now until then will seem about as long as the time from when you were"(A^2)\L"until now.
3 PRINT:PRINT"Do it again (Y/N)? ";:K=INSTR("NnYy",INPUT$(1)):IF K>2 THEN 1 ELSE IF K THEN SYSTEM ELSE 3
A compiled version of LOGTIME.BAS, LOGTIME.EXE, runs directly under all versions of MS-DOS (back at least to version 3) including DOS mode (or in a DOS window) of Windows 3.x and 9x. LOGTIME.EXE is in LOGTIME.ZIP along with ready-to-load versions of LOGTIME.BAS and LOGTIM.DO (and also this document). LOGTIME.ZIP may be downloaded from

http://ourworld.compuserve.com/homepages/jmkenney/logtime.zip
DN      CONTENTS     UP

Will Life Extension Help?



A popular topic these days is the extension of life span through nutrition or medical intervention, e.g., by genetic modification to allow more cell divisions.

Examine the highest ages on the tables presented above, or extrapolate past them to higher octaves, and you will see how far life must be extended to give even a small increase in subjective life span: even an added decade provides only a fraction of an octave. (Perform the calculations described immediately above, assuming increasingly higher ages at death, and see how the increase would be perceived by comparison with your past life.)

Only a truly astounding increase in years, approaching immortality, could provide a meaningful change (and the social consequences of such an increase could be devastating). An interesting speculation is what life would be like for really old immortals: would the sun be streaking across the heavens and the days flashing like a strobe, as seen by the time traveller in H. G. Wells' The Time Machine? And would their old memories be edited to provide room for new memories? (How: FIFO or selective?) Perhaps a finite memory would provide a limit to the Logtime shrinkage.

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Intergenerational Relationships



An important consequence of a knowledge of Logtime can be an appreciation of an important difference between the young and the old. While the elderly are commonly regarded as slow, by their own perceptions they are racing past the lives of the young: it is only the young who experience the "endless summers".

In the tables above, compare the years in your current octave with those of your children or parents (and grandchildren or grandparents). Try to see how these differences may affect communications between you. For example, what the young may think of as prudent planning for the future may be dismissed by the old as living for the present; the generations simply scale their lives differently.

DN      CONTENTS     UP

What is the Clock? (Unanswered Questions)



There are many questions evoked by the Logtime hypothesis: What is being compared? Is there a real biological clock counting the days, or is the flow of data into the brain used (analogous to an hourglass or a water clock)? If the latter, does it use raw or processed data, or some weighted combination? Are new experiences and familiar ones weighted differently? This has to involve the nature of memory. Is there a sense of elapsed time independent of memory?

An interval of time may be judged differently in retrospect than while being experienced; it can fly when you're short of time, but may seem long afterward if you accomplished much or had a memorable experience. How does one's health, particularly relating to memory, affect time perception? This is a rich, albeit difficult, field for investigation.

Return to text
DN      CONTENTS     UP

References



(biographical data added by present author)
William James (1842-1910; professor of philosophy at Harvard; "one of the founders of modern psychology"; "father of American psychology"): The Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt 1890.
This monumental work ("Perhaps the most important English-language psychology text in history.") has been placed online in its entirety (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/Principles/index.htm) (link above); Chapter XV: "The Perception of Time" (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/Principles/prin15.htm) is a lengthy treatise largely of short-term phenomena, but with an important discussion of the effect of aging:
"The same space of time seems shorter as we grow older -- that is, the days, the months, and the years do so; whether the hours do so is doubtful, and the minutes and seconds to all appearance remain about the same.
'Whoever counts many lustra in his memory need only question himself to find that the last of these, the past five years, have sped much more quickly than the preceding periods of equal amount. Let any one remember his last eight or ten school years: it is the space of a century. Compare with them the last eight or ten years of life: it is the space of an hour.'
So writes Prof. Paul Janet (1823-1899; from 1864 professor of philosophy at the Sorbonne) [Revue Philosophique, vol. III. p. 496], and gives a solution which can hardly be said to diminish the mystery. There is a law, he says, by which the apparent length of an interval at a given epoch of a man's life is proportional to the total length of the life itself. A child of 10 feels a year as 1/10 of his whole life -- a man of 50 as 1/50, the whole life meanwhile apparently preserving a constant length. This formula roughly expresses the phenomena, it is true, but cannot possibly be an elementary psychic law; and it is certain that, in great part at least, the foreshortening of the years as we grow older is due to the monotony of memory's content, and the consequent simplification of the backward-glancing view. In youth we may have an absolutely new experience, subjective or objective, every hour of the day. Apprehension is vivid, retentiveness strong, and our recollections of that time, like those of a time spent in rapid and interesting travel, are of something intricate, multitudinous, and long-drawn-out. But as each passing year converts some of this experience into automatic routine which we hardly note at all, the days and the weeks smooth themselves out in recollection to contentless units, and the years grow hollow and collapse."
Return to text

Only one publication introducing a logarithmic scale of time perception has been identified:
Rodney Collin (1909-1956): The Theory of Celestial Influence -- Man, The Universe, and Cosmic Mystery (1948). London: Stuart & Watkins 1971. New York: State Mutual Book 1981.
This book was cited by
Michael Shallis: On Time. London: Burnett Books 1982. New York: Schocken Books 1983.
As described by Shallis, Collin devised a logarithmic time scale based on lunar months, with 1, 10, 100, and 1000 lunar months (equally spaced on a linear scale labeled 0, 1, 2, and 3) roughly corresponding to, respectively, the dates of conception, birth, 7 years, and death (77 years), thereby dividing life into three equal parts: gestation, childhood, and maturity. (Maturity at 7?) After noting that "time seems to pass about ten times more slowly for a six year old child as for a sixty year old man", Shallis identifies the logarithmic scale with "metabolic rate and therefore to some extent with human experience."

Two relevant books, seen many years ago, could not be located. They both described a World War I French study of wound healing which concluded that the time for tissue to grow over a superficial wound of a given size was directly proportional to the age of the patient. At least one of these books suggested a common metabolic connection with changing time perception. The rapid healing of injuries in children is well known, but are there any modern studies quantifying this phenomenon?

Return to text

Ernst Heinrich Weber (1795-1878; professor at the University of Leipzig 1818-1871): De Tactu (On Touch) (1834; in Latin); and Der Tastsinn und das Gemeingefühl (The Sense of Touch and the Common Sensibility) (1851)("considered to be 'the foundation stone of experimental psychology'").

Gustav Theodor Fechner (1801-1887; physicist; professor of philosophy at Leipzig): Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel 1860. Bristol: Thoemmes Press 1999 (translation: Elements of Psychophysics, Volume I only. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston 1966.)
In the nineteenth century, a very general description of sensory perception was developed by Ernst Heinrich Weber and Gustav Theodor Fechner, known as "Weber's law", "Fechner's law", or the "Weber-Fechner law", etc. Weber found that, over a range of stimulus intensity, the minimum amount by which the stimulus must be changed for the change to be noticed is directly proportional to the stimulus. After rediscovering this relationship, Fechner showed that this indicates that the intensity of sensation is proportional to the logarithm of the intensity of the stimulus. A translation of Sections VII and XIV of Fechner's Elements may be found online (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Fechner/). At the same site there is also an introduction, by Robert H. Wozniak, to Fechner's Elements (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/Fechner/wozniak.htm).

There are many sites that discuss Weber-Fechner, such as Dennis Leri: MENTAL FURNITURE #10/The Fechner Weber Principle (www.semiophysics.com/menta10.htm).

Return to text
Return to Appendix

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Internet Links



(Some web links appear in References, immediately above.)

Generation Gap Calculator, By Edgar Matias
http://edgarmatias.com/gapcalc.html
This site, dated 1997 (earlier than the Logtime site), starts with the same premise as Logtime, and even uses startlingly similar phrases (see below), but does not use logarithms. Instead, there is an interactive program (not requiring script!) which produces tables of relative ages for a visitor-supplied pair of ages to be compared.
"Aging and Age Perception

Getting older? Feel like the years are flying by faster than they used to? Ever wonder why?
As you age, your perception of time changes. Every year, 1-year represents a smaller and smaller piece of your life.
To a two-year-old, 1 year is half their life. To a 10 yr old, a year is 10%; to a 20 yr old, only 5%.
Thus, for a 20 yr old to experience the age increase of a 10 yr old's year, (s)he would have to age by 10% or 2 years.
Using the Generation Gap Calculator (below), you can experiment with aging yourself like someone of a different age."


Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy:
The Experience and Perception of Time
http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/time-experience/


Time Keepers of the Calendar and Clock
http://www.ernie.cummings.net/time.htm
A colorful site about clocks and calendars and their histories.

DN      CONTENTS     UP

Appendix: Logtime Math



For those readers who have had some calculus, or at least are familiar with the elementary properties of logarithms, a derivation of the mathematical relationships of Logtime is presented below. What will be shown is that a simple, plausible assumption about the estimation of time intervals by the human mind ("psychochronometry") implies a mathematical model that can explain common observations, at least to the extent allowed by the unmeasurable subjective nature of those observations.

This is not a "proof" of Logtime: the basic premise is not only unproven but probably unprovable because of its subjective nature and the difficulty of experimentation. Long time intervals would be required for test subjects to age, and their aging could not be reversed! Note that the same problem exists for the linear alternative model. The best choice among psychochronometric cognitive models must ultimately depend upon which predicts consequences that seem the closest to actual human experience. As crudely describable as it may be, this experience seems at least to enable the logarithmic model to be selected over the linear one. (Are there any others?)

Note that the following analysis (developed independently) recapitulates the derivation of the "Weber-Fechner law" of sensory stimulation, with aging as the stimulus:
For human age, let

        t = objective (clock) time

and its dependent variable

        s = subjective (perceived) time

The Logtime hypothesis is that the length of a time interval is perceived as its proportion of current age. This can be expressed in differential form as

        ds = dt/t

where the constant of proportionality is made unity by proper choice of the (undefined) unit of s.

Integrating:

        s = ln(t) + c

For a finite increment of s, s to s', bounded by corresponding clock ages t and t':

        s' - s = ln(t') - ln(t)

               = ln(t'/t)

The perceived increment of subjective age is seen to depend only on the ratio of corresponding clock ages and not on their absolute values (or unit). All values of t and t' having a given ratio (e.g., t'/t = 2) yield the same value of (s' - s). The natural logarithm function (ln) relating these temporal increments (objective and subjective) suggested the name "Logtime".

Note that a consequence of this logarithmic relationship is that subjective increments add as clock increments (ratios) multiply. Consider a second subjective increment, s' to s", immediately following s to s'. The total increment, bounded by t and t", is

        s" - s = (s" - s') + (s' - s)

               = ln(t"/t') + ln(t'/t)

               = ln[(t"/t')*(t'/t)]

               = ln(t"/t)

The special case of doubling a subjective increment therefore requires a squaring of the clock age ratio. If that ratio is 2 (the octave), then two successive octaves are required, quadrupling the clock age.

Expressing the clock age ratio as a function of the corresponding subjective age increment:

        t'/t = exp(s' - s)

shows the exponential growth in relative clock age required for a linear (uniformly perceived) passage of subjective time.
Return to Logarithmic Explanation
Return to Scale of Life

DN      CONTENTS     UP

The Author



The author received the degrees of Bachelor of Electrical Engineering from The City College of New York and Master of Science in Electrical Engineering from Columbia University, and is now retired from an engineering career spent largely in the field of microwave semiconductor measurements.

The author's concept of Logtime, including the basic math, dates from the 1960's, inspired in part by the logarithmic scales on the slide rules then in use before the development of electronic calculators and personal computers, and also by the log scales on some types of graph paper used to plot data.

Recent searches (occasioned by writing this monograph) have revealed that the basic elements of Logtime were described by others much earlier, going back at least to the 19th century, although references are few and these ideas seem not to have achieved a general awareness, let alone acceptance.

In particular, Paul Janet may have been first to propose that current age is the basis for estimating time intervals, and Rodney Collin the earliest author to introduce a logarithmic subjective time scale (albeit a somewhat bizarre one), but no reference has been found making an explicit mathematical connection between these concepts. Gustav Theodor Fechner, however, used analogous math for physical stimuli. Logtime may therefore be regarded as an extension of the Weber-Fechner law, with aging as the stimulus. Has any such suggestion been published?

Additional references and reader reactions would be greatly appreciated. Please send to:

jmkenney@kafalas.com


The latest version of this document may be found at
http://ourworld.compuserve.com/homepages/jmkenney/
or downloaded in LOGTIME.ZIP from
http://ourworld.compuserve.com/homepages/jmkenney/logtime.zip

DN      CONTENTS     UP
...The Bird of Time has but a little way
To flutter--and the Bird is on the Wing.

...The Wine of Life keeps oozing drop by drop,
The Leaves of Life keep falling one by one.

Ah, but my Computations, People say,
Reduced the Year to better reckoning?--Nay,
'Twas only striking from the Calendar
Unborn To-morrow and dead Yesterday.

-- Rubáiyát of Omar Khayyám:
Edward FitzGerald

...Today will die tomorrow;
Time stoops to no man's lure...

-- The Garden of Proserpine:
Swinburne

DN      UP

Contents



Top
Opening Verse
Problems of Time Perception
Logtime: The Logarithmic Explanation
The Scale of Life
Logarithmic Lifetimes
The Last Judgement
Will Life Extension Help?
Intergenerational Relationships
What is the Clock? (Unanswered Questions)
References
Internet Links
Appendix: Logtime Math
The Author
Closing Verse

UP
Prose text Copyright © 2000-2002 James Main Kenney. All Rights Reserved.
Permission granted for non-commercial reproduction or reposting of unaltered page.
Revised 2002-03-16
Visits since 2000-11-12:

WebCounter